国際誌論文データベース

日本の社会心理学者たちは,活発な研究活動を展開・公表しており,その成果は日本語による論文であれば例えば日本社会心理学会の機関誌である「社会心理学研究」等の学会誌に掲載され,また学術書として公刊されています.一方,当然のことながら学問に国境はなく,特に近年では国際的な論文誌や書籍にその成果が掲載されることも増えてきました.しかし,こうした国際的成果をくまなく知ることは,あまりにそのフィールドが広いためにあまり容易ではありませんでした.

そこで,このページでは,日本の社会心理学者による国際的な研究活動の成果を広く共有・広報するために,日本社会心理学会会員による国際査読誌や書籍に掲載された学術論文(2013年以降に公刊されたもの)を,会員の皆様からの自薦・他薦の情報提供にもとづいて,あるいは,広報委員が不定期にPsycINFO, GoogleScholarなどを使って渉猟して,掲載しています.書誌情報は,メールニュース等の媒体でもご案内します.

  • 情報を掲載している論文を登録順(最新のものが上)でリストしています.
  • ページ右上「このサイトを検索」から,キーワード検索が可能です.

会員の皆様

掲載決定・刊行予定となった論文や書籍(分担執筆等も含む)をこちらのフォームから是非お知らせください.
既に登録した情報への追加・修正依頼は広報委員会( jssp_pr[at]googlegroups.com )([at]を@に変えてください。)に直接ご連絡ください.

現在の掲載論文数は,535件です.


Park, J., van den Broek, K.L., Bhullar, N., Ogunbode, C.A., Schermer, J.A., Doran, R., Ardi, R., Hanss, D., Maran, D.A., Aquino, S., Ayanian, A.H., Chegeni, R., Chukwuorji, J.C., Enea, V., Ghanbarian, E., Jian, F., Lins, S., Lomas, M.J., Lu, S., Marot, T., Mbungu, W., Navarro-Carrillo, G., Kehinde, O.A., Onyutha, C., Reyes, M.E.S., Salmela-Aro, K., Sollar, T., Tahir, H., Tan, C.S., Torres-Marin, J., Tsubakita, T., Volkodav, T., Wlodarczyk, A., & Yadav, R. (in press).

Park, J., van den Broek, K.L., Bhullar, N., Ogunbode, C.A., Schermer, J.A., Doran, R., Ardi, R., Hanss, D., Maran, D.A., Aquino, S., Ayanian, A.H., Chegeni, R., Chukwuorji, J.C., Enea, V., Ghanbarian, E., Jian, F., Lins, S., Lomas, M.J., Lu, S., Marot, T., Mbungu, W., Navarro-Carrillo, G., Kehinde, O.A., Onyutha, C., Reyes, M.E.S., Salmela-Aro, K., Sollar, T., Tahir, H., Tan, C.S., Torres-Marin, J., Tsubakita, T., Volkodav, T., Wlodarczyk, A., & Yadav, R. (in press).
Comparison of the Inter-item Correlations of the Big Five Inventory-10 (BFI-10) between Western and non-Western contexts.
Big Five Inventory-10(BFI-10)の項目間相関に関する欧米諸国と非欧米諸国の比較
Personality and Individual Differences, 196.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2022.111751

The Big Five Inventory-10 (BFI-10; Rammstedt & John, 2007) is one of many short versions of personality inventories that measure the Big Five trait dimensions. Short versions of scales often present methodological challenges as a trade-off for their convenience. Based on samples from 28 countries (N = 10,560), the current study investigated inter-item correlations estimated using Omega coefficients within each of the five personality characteristics measured by the BFI-10. Results showed that inter-item correlations were significantly lower, in the sample data from non-Western countries compared with the Western countries, for three of the five personality traits, specifically Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Emotional Stability. Our findings indicate that the psychometric challenges exist across different cultures and traits. We offer recommendations when using short-item scales such as BFI-10 in survey research.


Shimizu, Y., Hashimoto, T., & Karasawa, K. (2022).

Shimizu, Y.(清水佑輔), Hashimoto, T.(橋本剛明), & Karasawa, K.(唐沢かおり) (2022).
Ageist attitudes: Youth identity, subjective time to become older, and impressions of the general older population's physical and mental health.
エイジズム:若者アイデンティティ、高齢者になるまでの主観的時間、高齢者一般の身体的・精神的健康状態への印象との関連について
Advances in Gerontology, 12(2), 157-161. 
https://doi.org/10.1134/S2079057022020163

Reduction in ageist attitudes is crucial to resolve intergenerational conflicts between older adults and other generations. As factors related to ageist attitudes, the extent to which people perceive themselves to be young should be the focus. The perceived link between older adults and illness also has a significant relationship with ageist attitudes. This study examines the relationship between ageist attitudes and youth identity (the extent to which people feel they belong to the young population as opposed to the older population), and impressions of the physical and mental health of the general older population. The results of an online survey of Japanese participants (n = 474) showed that those with higher youth identity held stronger ageist attitudes. While impressions of the physical health of the general older population were not associated with ageist attitudes, participants with more positive impressions of older adults’ mental health had weaker ageist attitudes. Future research should extend our model by adding factors not considered in this study.


Kanemasa, Y., Asano, R, Komura, K., & Miyagawa, Y. (in press).

Kanemasa, Y. (金政祐司), Asano, R (浅野良輔), Komura, K. (古村健太郎), & Miyagawa, Y. (宮川裕基).  (in press)
longitudinal associations between personality traits and psychological intimate partner violence.
パーソナリティ特性と心理的IPVに関する縦断的関連性
Journal of Marriage and Family.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jomf.12869

Objective and background: This study examined the longitudinal adverse influences of attachment anxiety and the dark triad on psychological intimate partner violence (IPV) perpetration and victimization through daily negative emotions. Method: We conducted a 1-year paired longitudinal study that followed married couples across three waves of assessment. The participants were 471 married couples in Japan. They completed measures of the dark triad and attachment anxiety (Wave 1), daily negative emotions (Wave 2: 1 month after Wave 1), and psychological IPV victimization (Wave 3: 1 year after Wave 1). Results: The actor–partner interdependence model showed that attachment anxiety and the dark triad were positively associated with psychological IPV perpetration 1 year later through different processes. Whereas attachment anxiety was related to psychological IPV perpetration through daily negative emotions for both men and women, the dark triad was directly related to psychological IPV perpetration across sex groups. In addition, attachment anxiety was positively related to psychological IPV victimization through daily negative emotions for men and women, but the dark triad was not. Conclusion and implication: Attachment anxiety and the dark triad were associated differently with psychological IPV perpetration 1 year later, whereas these processes are equivalent for men and women. Our results imply that for individuals with high attachment anxiety, interventions to regulate daily negative emotions such as depression and distress may buffer against both IPV victimization and perpetration.

夫婦関係において、1)愛着不安は、日常的なネガティブ感情の蓄積を介して、親密なパートナーへの暴力に繋がること、2)ダークトライアドは直接的に親密なパートナーへの暴力に繋がることを、縦断データを用いたAPIMにより明らかにしました。


Kajimura, S., Nozaki, Y., Goto, T., & Smallwood, J. (2022).

Kajimura, S., Nozaki, Y.(野崎優樹), Goto, T.(後藤崇志), & Smallwood, J. (2022).
Not all daydreaming is equal: A longitudinal investigation of social and general daydreaming and marital relationship quality.
空想はすべて同じではない:社会的および一般的な空想と夫婦関係の質との関連についての縦断的調査
Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 904025.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.904025

Preliminary evidence suggests that daydreaming about other people has adaptive value in daily social lives. To address this possibility, we examined whether daydreaming plays a role in maintaining close, stable relationships using a 1-year prospective longitudinal study. We found that individuals’ propensity to daydream about their marital partner is separate to general daydreaming. In contrast to general daydreaming, which was associated with lower subsequent relationship investment size (i.e., magnitude and importance of resources attached to a relationship) in the marital partner, partner-related social daydreaming led to a greater subsequent investment size. Additionally, attachment styles moderated these effects. The effect of daydreaming regarding investment size was found only in securely attached individuals. This research advances the emerging field of social daydreaming and highlights self-generated thought as a critical tool that can help people navigate the complex social world.


Ogihara, Y., & Ito, A. (2022).

Ogihara, Y. (荻原祐二), & Ito, A. (伊藤篤希) (2022).
Unique names increased in Japan over 40 years: Baby names published in municipality newsletters show a rise in individualism, 1979-2018.
個性的な名前は40年間にわたって増加している:地方自治体の広報誌に掲載された新生児の名前は個人主義傾向の増加を示している, 1979-2018
Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, 3, 100046.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cresp.2022.100046 

We examined whether Japanese culture became more individualistic by investigating baby names published in municipality newsletters for 40 years between 1979 and 2018. Previous research has shown that the rates of unique names increased in Japan between 2004 and 2018, suggesting a rise in uniqueness-seeking and individualism. However, it was unclear whether the trend after the 2000s was found before the 2000s. To discuss cultural changes, it is desirable to examine trends over longer periods of time. Unlike in the U.S., databases of names given to newborns in previous years did not exist in Japan. Thus, we collected baby names published in municipality newsletters. Results showed that over the 40 years, unique names increased in all of the municipalities examined, indicating an increase in uniqueness-seeking and individualism in Japan. The current research investigated cultural changes for a longer time period and presented further evidence of rising individualism in Japan.

https://twitter.com/Yu_Ogihara/status/1540240452171239424

Wang, K., Goldenberg, A., Miller, J. K. et al. (2021).

Wang, K., Goldenberg, A., Miller, J. K. et al. (2021).
A multi-country test of brief reappraisal interventions on emotions during the COVID-19 pandemic.
[Registered Report] 新型コロナウィルス感染症流行下におけるネガティブ情動に対する情動制御方略の効果:超多国間実験による検証
Nature Human Behaviour, 5, 1089-1110. 
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01173-x

The COVID-19 pandemic has increased negative emotions and decreased positive emotions globally. Left unchecked, these emotional changes might have a wide array of adverse impacts. To reduce negative emotions and increase positive emotions, we tested the effectiveness of reappraisal, an emotion-regulation strategy that modifies how one thinks about a situation. Participants from 87 countries and regions (n = 21,644) were randomly assigned to one of two brief reappraisal interventions (reconstrual or repurposing) or one of two control conditions (active or passive). Results revealed that both reappraisal interventions (vesus both control conditions) consistently reduced negative emotions and increased positive emotions across different measures. Reconstrual and repurposing interventions had similar effects. Importantly, planned exploratory analyses indicated that reappraisal interventions did not reduce intentions to practice preventive health behaviours. The findings demonstrate the viability of creating scalable, low-cost interventions for use around the world.

調査票の日本語訳および日本データの収集を山田祐樹(九州大学)・石井辰典(日本女子大学)・国里愛彦(専修大学)・角南直幸(Delaware Data Innovation Lab)・井隼経子(福岡工業大学)が担当した。この活動に際し、日本心理学会の「新型コロナウイルス感染拡大に関連した実践活動及び研究」の助成を受けた。


Psychological Science Accelerator Self-Determination Theory Collaboration (2022).

Psychological Science Accelerator Self-Determination Theory Collaboration (2022).
A global experiment on motivating social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic.
社会的距離拡大方略を動機づけるものは何か?新型コロナウィルス流行下における超多国間実験
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(22), e2111091119.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2111091119

Finding communication strategies that effectively motivate social distancing continues to be a global public health priority during the COVID-19 pandemic. This cross-country, preregistered experiment (n = 25,718 from 89 countries) tested hypotheses concerning generalizable positive and negative outcomes of social distancing messages that promoted personal agency and reflective choices (i.e., an autonomy-supportive message) or were restrictive and shaming (i.e., a controlling message) compared with no message at all. Results partially supported experimental hypotheses in that the controlling message increased controlled motivation (a poorly internalized form of motivation relying on shame, guilt, and fear of social consequences) relative to no message. On the other hand, the autonomy-supportive message lowered feelings of defiance compared with the controlling message, but the controlling message did not differ from receiving no message at all. Unexpectedly, messages did not influence autonomous motivation (a highly internalized form of motivation relying on one’s core values) or behavioral intentions. Results supported hypothesized associations between people’s existing autonomous and controlled motivations and self-reported behavioral intentions to engage in social distancing. Controlled motivation was associated with more defiance and less long-term behavioral intention to engage in social distancing, whereas autonomous motivation was associated with less defiance and more short- and long-term intentions to social distance. Overall, this work highlights the potential harm of using shaming and pressuring language in public health communication, with implications for the current and future global health challenges.

調査票の日本語訳および日本データの収集を山田祐樹(九州大学)・石井辰典(日本女子大学)・国里愛彦(専修大学)・角南直幸(Delaware Data Innovation Lab)・井隼経子(福岡工業大学)が担当した。またこの活動に際し、日本心理学会「新型コロナウイルス感染拡大に関連した実践活動及び研究」の助成を受けた。


Ishii, T., & Watanabe, K. (2022).

Ishii, T.(石井辰典), & Watanabe, K. (2022).
Do empathetic people have strong religious beliefs? Survey studies with large Japanese samples.
共感的な人々は固い宗教的信念を持つだろうか?:大規模日本人サンプルを用いた検証
The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2022.2057059

The exploration of personality factors to explain individual differences in religiosity has demonstrated a link between empathic concern and religious beliefs using the Empathic Concern subscale of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI-EC). Research in the cognitive science of religion emphasized the role of empathizing ability related to mentalizing in acquisition of religious belief and has demonstrated the relationship between the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and religious belief. The current study was designed to compare the strength of relationships between religious belief and two representative measures of empathy (the IRI-EC and the EQ). Study 1 aimed to statistically evaluate the strength of the relationship between the EQ/IRI-EC and religious belief with four Japanese samples (Ns = 207, 155, 208, 183). The mini meta-analysis results with random effect model indicated that the effect size (semi partial correlation, r_sp) of the IRI-EC (r_sp = .120, 95%CI [.0002, .237]) was larger than that of the EQ (r_sp = .074, 95%CI [−.0001, .147]). Moreover, these results were confirmed by Study 2 (N = 1440). Thus, the present study provided reliable evidence of the link between empathy and religious belief in non-Western samples. We discuss how empathic concern and mentalizing-related empathy contribute to acquiring religious beliefs.


Lee, S., Shimizu, H., & Nakashima, K. I. (2022).

Lee, S. (李受珉), Shimizu, H., & Nakashima, K. I (中島健一郎). (2022). 
Shift-and-Persist Strategy: Tendencies and Effect on Japanese Parents and Children's Mental Health.
Shift-and-Persist Strategy: 日本人親子の類似性とメンタルヘルスへの影響
Japanese Psychological Research
https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12421

Low socioeconomic status has various adverse effects on health, which can be mitigated through the shift-and-persist (S-P) strategy. Studies have focused on how this strategy can affect health in the face of adversity. However, that children learn this strategy from positive role models, such as parents, is an unexamined precondition of the theory. This study presents one bit of supporting evidence for this precondition by examining the similarity in S-P among parent–child dyads using the actor-partner interdependence model. We also examine parent and child strategies related to depressive tendencies based on mixed results in relevant research. The results from 309 parent–child pairs indicate that shifting and persisting tendencies and depressive tendencies were similar among the parent–child pairs. Furthermore, regardless of their socioeconomic status, the parents’ and children’s persisting scores predicted lower levels of depressive tendencies as actor effects. Although this study does not fully support S-P theory, it provides important insights regarding similar patterns of strategic tendencies between parents and children and highlights the importance of positive role models.


Shimizu, Y., Osaki, S., Hashimoto, T., & Karasawa, K. (2022).

Shimizu, Y. (清水佑輔), Osaki, S., Hashimoto, T. (橋本剛明), & Karasawa, K. (唐沢かおり) (2022).
Social acceptance of smart city projects: Focus on the Sidewalk Toronto case.
スマートシティにおける社会的受容:トロントのケースに着目して
Frontiers in Environmental Science, 10, 898922. 
https://doi.org/10.3389/fenvs.2022.898922

We focused on Sidewalk Toronto, where the lack of trust in the business operators declined the social acceptance of the project. There are two main reasons for selecting Sidewalk Toronto as the focus of this research. First, Sidewalk Toronto has been extensively discussed in previous studies as a representative example of a large-scale smart city project whose decline in social acceptance may have caused its cancellation. Second, although Sidewalk Toronto is a public utility, the development involved the sister company of Google, a global data giant with a head office outside of Canada. As a result, to increase the social acceptance of smart city projects, we found that business operators should rigorously handle individual data, transparently implement projects, and provide an appropriate scope for the public authority. This research provides new perspectives for a wide range of research areas (e.g., environmental science, urban planning, and psychology) that aim to enhance the social acceptance of smart city projects. This research is also meaningful for business operators who work with the community. Future research will compare a variety of cases using interviews with citizens residing in cities with failed/successful smart city projects.